Subliminal Priming with Positive Auditory Stimuli Significantly Improves Self-Esteem and Emotional Well-Being
##plugins.themes.bootstrap3.article.main##
This retrospective study investigated the effect of positive auditory masked subliminal priming on 535 adult and child patients who presented for treatment at the Brain Wellness Spa (BWS) in Australia between the years of 2018 and 2021. Patients were assessed prior to first treatment session, using 7 self-administered questionnaires: a 33-question assessment of their state of being, a 10-question evaluation of emotion, a 12-question survey of long-term state, the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-21, the Penn State Worry Questionnaire, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, and the Brief Aggression Questionnaire. Subjects were then exposed to either 12 or 14 weekly treatment sessions composed of 25-minute masked subliminal auditory priming scripts. The scripts consisted of dozens of affirmational statements aimed at decreasing the subjects’ stress to emotional triggers and promoting psychological and emotional resilience. Patients were unaware of the subliminal messages underlying the music (auditory mask) during each treatment session. Patients completed identical questionnaires as follow-up after either their 11th or 13th treatment session. Statistical analysis of pre- and post-treatment questionnaires revealed significant improvement in subjective reports of self-esteem, depression, anxiety, stress, worry, and state of being. Despite these positive conclusions, results need to be interpreted taking into consideration the study limitations. However, based upon these conclusions, repetitive masked subliminal auditory priming therapy needs to be further investigated with larger sample sizes.
Downloads
References
-
Nguyen D, Wright E, Dedding C, Pham T, Bunders J. Low self-esteem and its association with anxiety, depression, and suicidal ideation in Vietnamese secondary school students: A cross-sectional study. Front Psychiatry. 2019; (10): 698
DOI | Google Scholar
1
-
Trzesniewski K, Donnellan M, Robins R. Stability of self-esteem across the life span. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003; 84(1): 205-220.
DOI | Google Scholar
2
-
Crocker J, Knight K. Contingencies of self-worth. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2005; 14(4): 200-203.
DOI | Google Scholar
3
-
Leary MR, Baumeister RF. The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2000; 32(1): 1-62.
DOI | Google Scholar
4
-
Martín-Albo J, Núñez J, Navarro J, Grijalvo F. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Translation and validation in university students. Span. J. Psychol. 2007; 10(2): 458-467.
DOI | Google Scholar
5
-
Conley J. The hierarchy of consistency: A review and model of longitudinal findings on adult individual differences in intelligence, personality and self-opinion. Pers. Individ. Differ. 1984; 5(1): 11-25.
DOI | Google Scholar
6
-
Kernis M, Grannemann B, Barclay L. Stability of self-esteem: Assessment, correlates, and excuse making. J Pers. 1992; 60(3): 621-644.
DOI | Google Scholar
7
-
Heatherton T, Polivy J. Development and validation of a scale for measuring state self-esteem. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991; 60(6): 895-910.
DOI | Google Scholar
8
-
Vall-Roqué H, Andrés A, Saldaña C. The impact of COVID-19 lockdown on social network sites use, body image disturbances and self-esteem among adolescent and young women. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2021; 110: 110293.
DOI | Google Scholar
9
-
Berthelot N, Lemieux R, Garon-Bissonnette J, Drouin-Maziade C, Martel É, Maziade M. Uptrend in distress and psychiatric symptomatology in pregnant women during the Coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic. Obstet Anesth Dig. 2021; 41(2): 80-81.
DOI | Google Scholar
10
-
Liu N, Zhang F, Wei C, Jia Y, Shang Z, Sun L, et al. Prevalence and predictors of PTSS during COVID-19 outbreak in China hardest-hit areas: Gender differences matter. Psychiatry Res. 2020; 287: 112921.
DOI | Google Scholar
11
-
Killgore W, Cloonan S, Taylor E, Anlap I, Dailey N. Increasing aggression during the COVID-19 lockdowns. J Affect Disord. 2021; 5: 100163.
DOI | Google Scholar
12
-
Rosenberg M, Schooler C, Schoenbach C, Rosenberg F. Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: Different concepts, different outcomes. Am Sociol Rev. 1995; 60(1): 141.
DOI | Google Scholar
13
-
Murphy C, Stosny S, Morrel T. Change in self-esteem and physical aggression during treatment for partner violent men. J Fam Violence. 2005; 20(4): 201-210.
DOI | Google Scholar
14
-
van Gaal S, de Lange F, Cohen M. The role of consciousness in cognitive control and decision making. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012; 6: 121.
DOI | Google Scholar
15
-
Dehaene S, Changeux J, Naccache L, Sackur J, Sergent C. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006; 10(5): 204-211.
DOI | Google Scholar
16
-
Kiefer M, Spitzer M. Time course of conscious and unconscious semantic brain activation. Neuroreport. 2000 11(11): 2401-2407.
DOI | Google Scholar
17
-
Sadaghiani S, Hesselmann G, Kleinschmidt A. Antagonistic contributions of distributed ongoing activity fluctuations to auditory stimulus detection. Neuroimage. 2009; 29(42): 13410-13417.
DOI | Google Scholar
18
-
Suslow T, Roestel C, Droste T, Arolt V. Automatic processing of verbal emotion stimuli in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2003; 120(2): 131-144.
DOI | Google Scholar
19
-
Koschack J, Höschel K, Irle E. Differential impairments of facial affect priming in subjects with acute or partially remitted major depressive episodes. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2003; 191(3): 175-181.
DOI | Google Scholar
20
-
Greenwald A, Draine S, Abrams R. Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science. 1996; 273(5282): 1699-1702.
DOI | Google Scholar
21
-
Ferrand L. The masked repetition priming effect dissipates when increasing the inter-stimulus interval: Evidence from word naming. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1996; 91(1): 15-25.
DOI | Google Scholar
22
-
Lowery B, Eisenberger N, Hardin C, Sinclair S. Long-term effects of subliminal priming on academic performance. SSRN Electronic Journal. 2006.
DOI | Google Scholar
23
-
Levy B, Pilver C, Chung P, Slade M. Subliminal strengthening: Improving elders' physical function over time through an implicit-age-stereotype intervention. Psychological WebScience. 2014; 25(12): 2127-2135.
DOI | Google Scholar
24
-
Farooqui A, Manly T. Anticipatory control through associative learning of subliminal relations: Invisible may be better than visible. Psychol Sci. 2015; 26(3): 325-334.
DOI | Google Scholar
25
-
Dijksterhuis A, Bos M, Nordgren L, van Baaren R. On making the right choice: The deliberation-without-attention effect. Science. 2006; 311(5763): 1005-1007.
DOI | Google Scholar
26
-
Dijksterhuis A. Think different: The merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision making. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004; 87(5): 586-598.
DOI | Google Scholar
27
-
Silverman L, Weinberger J. Mommy and I are one: Implications for psychotherapy. Am Psychol. 1985; 40(12): 1296-1308.
DOI | Google Scholar
28
-
Orbach I, Shopen-Kofman R, Mikulincer M. The impact of subliminal symbiotic vs identification messages in reducing anxiety. J Res Pers. 1994; 28(4): 492-504.
DOI | Google Scholar
29
-
Parkitny L, McAuley J. The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS). J Physiother. 2010; 56(3): 204.
DOI | Google Scholar
30
-
Gomez F. A guide to the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS 21). Umab.it. [Internet] [cited 2022 January 10] Available from: https://umab.it/wp-content/uploads/2016/10/DASS-21-GUIDE.pdf
Google Scholar
31
-
van Rijsoor S, Emmelkamp P, Vervaeke G. The Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: Structure, reliability and validity. Clin Psychol Psychother. 1999; 6(4): 297-307.
DOI | Google Scholar
32
-
Supple A, Su J, Plunkett S, Peterson G, Bush K. Factor structure of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2012; 44(5): 748-764.
DOI | Google Scholar
33
-
Webster G, DeWall C, Pond R, Deckman T, Jonason P, Le B, et al. The brief aggression questionnaire: Psychometric and behavioral evidence for an efficient measure of trait aggression. Aggressive Behavior. 2013; 40(2): 120-139.
DOI | Google Scholar
34
-
D'Agostino R, Belanger A, D'Agostino R. A suggestion for using powerful and informative tests of normality. Am Stat. 1990; 44(4): 316.
DOI | Google Scholar
35
-
D'Agostino R. An omnibus test of normality for moderate and large size samples. Biometrika. 1971; 58(2): 341-348.
DOI | Google Scholar
36
-
D'Agostino R, Pearson E. Tests for departure from normality. Empirical results for the distributions of b 2 and √b 1. Biometrika. 1973; 60(3): 613.
DOI | Google Scholar
37
-
Edgell S, Noon S. Effect of violation of normality on the t test of the correlation coefficient. Psychol Bull. 1984; 95(3): 576-583.
DOI | Google Scholar
38
-
Akoglu H. User's guide to correlation coefficients. Turk J Emerg Med, 2018; 18(3): 91-93.
DOI | Google Scholar
39
-
Orbach I. Self-destructive processes and suicide. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci. 2007; 44(4): 266–279.
Google Scholar
40
-
Webster G, DeWall C, Pond R, Deckman T, Jonason P, Le B, et al. The brief aggression questionnaire: Structure, validity, reliability, and generalizability. J Pers Assess. 2015; 97(6): 638-649.
DOI | Google Scholar
41
-
Greenwald, A. Within-subjects designs: To use or not to use?. Psychol Bull. 1976; 83(2): 314-320.
DOI | Google Scholar
42
-
APA Dictionary of Psychology. Practice Effect [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2020 January 11]; Available from: https://dictionary.apa.org/practice-effect.
Google Scholar
43
-
Daltrozzo J, Claude L, Tillmann B, Bastuji H, Perrin F. Working memory is partially preserved during sleep. Plos ONE. 2012; 7(12): e50997.
DOI | Google Scholar
44
-
Arzi A, Shedlesky L, Ben-Shaul M, Nasser K, Oksenberg A, Hairston I, Sobel N. Humans can learn new information during sleep. Nat Neurosci. 2012; 15(10): 1460-1465.
DOI | Google Scholar
45
-
Kouider S, de Gardelle V, Dehaene S, Dupoux E, Pallier C. Cerebral bases of subliminal speech priming. Neuroimage. 2010; 49(1): 922-929.
DOI | Google Scholar
46
-
Ruch S, Koenig T, Mathis J, Roth C, Henke K. Word encoding during sleep is suggested by correlations between word-evoked up-states and post-sleep semantic priming. Front Psychol. 2014; 5.
DOI | Google Scholar
47
-
Monahan J, Murphy S, Zajonc R. Subliminal mere exposure: Specific, general, and diffuse effects. Psychol Sci. 2000; 11(6): 462-466.
DOI | Google Scholar
48
-
Reuss H, Kiesel A, Kunde W, Hommel B. Unconscious activation of task sets. Conscious Cogn. 2011; 20(3): 556-567.
DOI | Google Scholar
49